The Chronicle of Higher Education
From the issue dated December 13, 2002


http://chronicle.com/weekly/v49/i16/16b01001.htm

Is Sociobiology Ready for Prime Time?

By NORA S. NEWCOMBE

Steven Pinker's new book, The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature (Viking)), consists of 509 pages explaining why we must be serious about human nature and extolling the virtues of the kind of theorizing often called sociobiology. In particular, sociobiologists think that many aspects of animal and human behavior can be understood as biologically based tendencies that evolved in order to increase an individual's chances of passing his or her genes on to subsequent generations. Pinker notes in his preface the reactions he got from his colleagues when he announced what his book was going to be about: "Not another book on nature and nurture! Are there really people out there who still believe that the mind is a blank slate?" Although his colleagues obviously didn't dissuade Pinker from finishing the book, I think they have a point.

The pendulum of popular and scholarly thinking has already swung away from the standard social-science model that Pinker criticizes, the one that assumed the primacy of culture. For example, magazines frequently show pictures of brains studied while their owners work at various tasks, along with text implying that biology determines the differences among people in the levels of activity in various areas of the brain. Sociobiology seems, in fact, already to constitute the new standard paradigm.

Given that predominance, two questions must be asked about the enterprise. First, is sociobiology up to the task of being a paradigm for productive scientific research, or do its explanations for human behavior lack the bread-and-butter proof that empirical scientists seek to provide? Second, does sociobiologists' fascination with biological causes lead them to ignore the possibilities of plasticity and change in human behavior?

Let's take a look at one research area to illustrate those issues. Sex differences in spatial ability are frequently the source of cocktail-party conversation and entertaining cartoons. We are all familiar with the stereotypes of the man who forges intrepidly into the wilderness armed only with an innate sense of direction, and the woman who holds maps upside down and depends on the kindness of strangers.

Nor are those images entirely false. Studies using standardized tests support the belief that men are markedly better than women at some spatial tasks. For example, the ability of the average male to mentally rotate the image of a three-dimensional object exceeds the ability of the average female by at least one standard deviation. For the statistically uninitiated, that large a difference is likely to be obvious to the casual observer; it is a difference one hardly needs science to reveal. Why are men and women so different? Are the differences set in stone?

Sociobiologists have a ready explanation for the sexes' different spatial abilities. Actually, they have two explanations. Both focus on the reproductive advantage that men with good spatial ability might have over other men, an advantage that would not apply to women.

One explanation focuses on Man the Hunter. Men are generally the hunters in hunting-gathering societies, and spatial skill is important in several components of hunting, including making weapons, tracking animals, and aiming the weapons at them. The protein that a man can get from hunting and give to his children helps ensure their survival, and prowess in hunting may also enhance a man's access to women (who wish to have a skilled hunter to provide for their children). In addition, a man's ability to aim may come in handy in any struggles for dominance with other males over sexual access to females.

The second explanation focuses on the Man Who Gets Around. Men need spatial skill to navigate around the territory so as to find as many fertile women as possible. There is in fact beautiful observational and experimental evidence of that kind of effect in the vole, a small mammal. One species, the prairie vole, forms pair bonds. A very similar species, the meadow vole, has a mating system in which females occupy territories; during the mating season, males make the rounds of females, attempting to reach as many as possible in time to impregnate them.

Strikingly, spatial ability (assessed by the ability to navigate mazes) is equal for males and females in the pair-bonded prairie vole, but male meadow voles beat females at spatial tasks during the mating season, when the part of their brain that supports navigation (the hippocampus) actually enlarges to meet the reproductive challenge.

Both Man the Hunter and the Man Who Gets Around seem sensible explanations of a sex difference in spatial ability. But rather than be dazzled by their surface plausibility, shouldn't we take a close look at their assumptions, and see to what extent the links in their arguments are supported (or undermined) by empirical proof? When we do that, we see that neither explanation is convincing.

Looking first at Man the Hunter, we note that although he may need spatial skill, so does Woman the Gatherer. Gathering may require long trips away from home to find various kinds of edible plants in their ripening seasons. True, animals move while vegetation sits still, but much hunting by our ancestors -- far from the swiftest creatures around -- may have consisted of setting traps, waiting at water holes, and other stationary activities, rather than tracking animals over meandering paths. In terms of manufacturing artifacts to use in hunting and gathering, spatial skill is just as necessary in weaving baskets or making pottery as it is in fashioning arrows and spearheads. Lastly, while the aiming component of hunting is a kind of action in space, it does not appear to be a spatial skill. Studies of aiming have found no correlation between success in hitting a target and success in rotating an image mentally.

Now take a look at the Man Who Gets Around. That explanation ignores one important fact: Women, unlike female meadow voles, live in social groups rather than occupying widely separated home territories. The skill it takes to impregnate many females probably relies more on abilities like charm and stealth than on the ability to find one's way among a cluster of huts.

The sociobiological explanations of sex differences in spatial ability raise another question, one that is inspired by sociobiology itself. What is the evolutionary advantage in a difference between the sexes in a trait that could benefit both sexes, when the trait carries no obvious metabolic cost? Most sex-specific traits that enhance reproduction produce antlers or ornamental tails, objects that are cumbersome and costly for the body to produce, and that a male has only because they enhance his ability to do combat with other males or to attract females. There is no reason for both sexes not to have a cheap-to-produce trait that is useful in a wide variety of settings.

So why is there a sex difference in spatial ability? One clue comes from a set of observations that sociobiologists often take to support their explanations -- namely, data that indicate (although sometimes inconsistently) that spatial ability within each sex fluctuates with hormone levels. Women often show better spatial skills when they are menstruating. Men with relatively low testosterone levels often have better spatial skills than other men.

But although correlations with hormone levels support some kind of biological explanation, they don't fit in the sociobiological explanations. There is no obvious reproductive advantage for women to have enhanced spatial skill at the infertile part of their cycle, and no obvious reason why spatial skill should vary at all with testosterone level for men -- or why the relationship is inverse rather than direct, as is the case for other behavioral traits like aggression. (One could argue that if a man can't compete physically with other men for mates, he needs spatial ability to find women, but once again that ignores the key fact about humans' living in social groups.)

Sex differences in spatial ability may exist because they are only accidentally linked to hormone levels -- they are a "spandrel," to use Stephen Jay Gould's term. After all, hormones account both for the appearance of acne at adolescence and for the fact that acne is typically more severe for males than females, but no one would suggest that acne confers a reproductive advantage. Acne is an example of a trait with a sex difference that is almost certainly accidental, and spatial ability may well be another.

Now let's turn to the second worry I raised about sociobiology -- that fascination with biological explanations may divert public attention from the possibility of malleability. The role of nature in setting parameters for human behavior does not exclude a substantial role for the environment in determining what behaviors are exhibited and what differences in behavior are observed.

Spatial ability again provides an example. Even though sex differences in the trait are substantial, levels of spatial ability do not seem to be biologically fixed. Spatial ability increases with schooling and with training and practice on specific tasks. Like other intellectual abilities, spatial ability has increased in the past century faster than any gene could change; that general increase has been called the Flynn effect after its discoverer, the political scientist James R. Flynn.

If we want to maximize the human capital available for occupations that draw on spatial skill, like mathematics, engineering, architecture, physics, chemistry, geology, and computer science, we would do better to concentrate on learning how to improve people's spatial abilities, rather than trying to understand the sex difference.

My point is not that sociobiology is wrong or evil, nor that we should return to the standard social-science paradigm. Rather, now that we have accepted that human nature is not a blank slate, we should carefully investigate what is written on it using our accepted standards of evidence, proof, and logic. We can expect that, as is true in all of science, some sociobiological explanations will be upheld (some already have been), while others will be found wanting. What we all want to read, eventually, is the book entitled What's Inscribed on the Human Slate, and What We Can and Cannot Do About It.

Nora S. Newcombe is a professor of psychology at Temple University. Her most recent book is Making Space: The Development of Spatial Representation and Reasoning (MIT Press, 2000), written with Janellen Huttenlocher.

http://chronicle.com
Section: The Chronicle Review
Volume 49, Issue 16, Page B10

Front page | Career Network | Search | Site map | Help

Copyright © 2002 by The Chronicle of Higher Education