



 Front Page
 Today's News
 Information Technology
 Teaching
 Publishing
 Money
 Government &
Politics
 Community Colleges
 Science
 Students
 Athletics
 International
 People
 Events
 The Chronicle Review
 Jobs

 Colloquy
 Colloquy Live
 Magazines &
Journals
 New Grant Competitions
 Facts & Figures
 Issues in Depth
 Site Sampler

 This Week's Issue
 Back Issues
 Related Materials

 About The Chronicle
 How to Contact Us
 How to Register
 How to Subscribe
 Subscriber Services
 Change Your User Name  Change Your Password
 Forgot Your Password?
 How to Advertise
 Press Inquiries
 Corrections
 Privacy Policy
 The Mobile Chronicle
 Help
|

|
From the issue dated April 25,
2003
|
|
Every Unhappy Family Has Its Own Bilinear Influence
FunctionResearchers propose a mathematical model of
marriage
By DAVID GLENN
A few years ago, an
unhappily married couple identified as "Angie" and "Dave"
visited the University
of Washington at Seattle's Family Research Laboratory, better known
on that campus as the Love Lab. Using techniques he first developed
in 1979, John M. Gottman, a professor emeritus of psychology there,
recorded the couple as they had a difficult conversation, carefully
noting their emotional signals each time they spoke.
Dave:
"We got to keep the communication lines open, we got to try to
figure out how we can work on it. Some bills we got we can't pay
off."
Angie: "The bills aren't the issue."
Dave:
"Usually it's how can I try to please you and how you"
Angie:
"No, I can please myself. I get happiness from my job, I get
happiness from work, I get happiness from you, but I feel stifled
..."
In the last 24 years, Mr. Gottman and his colleagues
have recorded thousands of such conversations, using careful
techniques to measure and notate the participants' emotions each
step of the way. After Angie and Dave's talk, Mr. Gottman says, his
assistants reviewed a videotape, scoring each sentence and facial
expression on such measures as disgust (-3), affection (+4), whining
(-1), and contempt (-4). (Angie's grimace as she said "The bills
aren't the issue" was scored as contempt.)
During the 1980s,
Mr. Gottman gained prominence by mining such laboratory data for
insights into the quandaries of marriage. He has argued, for
example, that a crucial predictor of divorce is a husband's
inability or unwillingness to be influenced by his wife's
suggestions and emotional expressions. This husbandly tone-deafness,
he says, augurs divorce much more strongly than, say, the general
level of anger expressed in the marriage. In longitudinal studies of
couples who have passed through the Love Lab, Mr. Gottman and his
colleagues have successfully predicted which ones will divorce with
greater than 90-percent accuracy.
Despite those successes, in
the early 1990s Mr. Gottman found himself feeling anxious about his
scholarly legacy. "My wife was pregnant, and we had one of those
heart-to-heart talks," he recalls. "I said, I'm just not going to
feel complete unless I can get back to my mathematical roots. I
didn't really have respect for my work unless I could put it on a
mathematical footing." (Mr. Gottman intended to earn a Ph.D. in
mathematics at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology in the late
1960s, but switched fields because he found his psychologist
roommate's textbooks to be much more interesting than his
own.)
A decade later, Mr. Gottman believes he has found that
footing. In The Mathematics of Marriage: Dynamic Nonlinear
Models (MIT Press), which he wrote in collaboration with four
mathematicians, Mr. Gottman uses the tools of calculus to describe
the interactions of couples like Angie and Dave. The models
presented in the book, he says, offer insights into the heaven and
hell of couplehood that he would never have found by sifting through
his data with standard linear statistical tools. He has already
begun to apply those insights in his therapeutic work
-- including with Angie and Dave themselves, whose
conversations are transcribed in the book.
The Stars Were
Aligned
The germ of The Mathematics of Marriage
was a remarkable piece of luck. Around the time of the
heart-to-heart conversation with his wife, Mr. Gottman forgot to
send in a reply card to a scientific book-of-the-month club, and
therefore received a book he'd never heard of: Mathematical
Biology (Springer-Verlag, 1989), by James D. Murray, a professor
of mathematics at the University of Oxford. The book explains how to
use nonlinear equations to illuminate the mechanics of complex
dynamic systems, such as the growth of brain tumors.
"The
book was so different from all the other books I'd been reading in
applied mathematics," Mr. Gottman says. "Finally, concepts like
catastrophe theory were very clear. I understood what it all meant."
Mr. Gottman sent a letter to Mr. Murray in Oxford -- but the
reply came from just five blocks away. Mr. Murray had retired early
from Oxford and moved to Seattle; he was teaching at Mr. Gottman's
own university.
The two men met for lunch. "I thought John
Gottman's ideas about having mathematics involved [in his marriage
studies] were ridiculous, and I told him that," recalls Mr. Murray.
"But by the end of the lunch, when I saw what he had in mind, I was
totally hooked."
A similar process ensued when Mr. Murray
recruited some of his graduate students to work on the project.
"They raised their eyebrows," he says. Six months later, however,
"you couldn't go into the lounge in applied mathematics without
getting drawn into arguments about how to write these marriage
equations. It sort of took over the department for a while." (Three
of those students are, with Mr. Gottman and Mr. Murray, the book's
authors: Kristin R. Swanson, now a research assistant professor of
mathematics in the department of pathology at the University of
Washington Medical Center; Catherine C. Swanson, now a software
engineer at the university; and Rebecca Tyson, now an assistant
professor of mathematics at Okanagan University College, in Kelowna,
British Columbia.)
What the students were modeling in that
lounge was not "marriage" per se, but the dynamics of marital
conversations. Before looking at data from any real-world couples,
they began with some very simple hypotheses: the idea, for example,
that spouses will react emotionally to the most recent comment made
by their partners. At this early stage they sketched crude
"influence functions" -- calculus equations that described a
dynamic system in which a snarky comment by one spouse would result
in negative emotions in the partner, sometimes resulting in a
downward spiral. When they tested those first equations against the
Love Lab's data, however, they did not match at all.
The
scholars soon realized that they needed to add a constant that
represented each partner's "uninfluenced steady state" -- that
is, the person's general level of cheerfulness or gloom, independent
of the spouse's behavior on a particular day. "In retrospect, we
should have thought of that at the very beginning," says Mr. Murray.
"But once we added that constant, everything fell in just
beautifully."
Mood-Altering Powers
The
researchers now had equations that fit Mr. Gottman's data quite
closely. They used these to develop bilinear influence functions,
which describe a person's ability to affect his or her spouse's
mood. Some couples, which Mr. Gottman calls "volatile," routinely
unleash anger at one another -- but offset that anger with even
larger doses of warm feelings. Those couples tend to be stable and
successful, Mr. Gottman says, because they are able to influence one
another with both anger and affection. (The influence functions of
"conflict avoiding" couples look very different.)
The
scholars also constructed a more sophisticated variant, using
equations known as ojive functions. This second model incorporates
tipping points and threshold effects. "The idea here," says Ms.
Tyson, "is that you have to be negative enough before you
make your partner negative, and positive enough before you make your
partner positive." Ojive functions fit the Love Lab's data better
still. "All of a sudden we had a language for describing what was
going on that appeared to be quite universal," says Mr.
Gottman.
A final innovation came in 1998, when the team added
the concepts of "repair" and "damping" to their models. Those terms
refer to spouses' conscious attempts to turn difficult conversations
in positive directions (repair) or negative ones (damping). Repair
attempts typically include jokes, soothing comments, or changes of
subject; damping generally involves making hurtful comments even
when one's partner is clearly trying to be positive. "Without repair
and damping, we knew that the bilinear influence functions weren't
going to work," says Kristin Swanson, "because the equations allowed
people to just spontaneously combust with happiness or spontaneously
combust with unhappiness. The repair and damping concepts don't
completely fix that, but they mediate it."
So what's the cash
value of all this mathematics? What insights has it offered Mr.
Gottman that he couldn't have derived through ordinary intuition, or
by analyzing his data with humdrum statistical techniques? "I
believe it was Lord Rutherford who said, If you need to use
statistics, then you should design a better experiment," says Mr.
Murray. "Statistics will just give you the bare facts. If you want
to understand why a dynamic system behaves as it does, then
you need to use nonlinear tools."
Perhaps the most important
insight generated by these marriage models is that particular
couples tend to have more than one style of emotional connection. In
1968, the theoretical biologist Ludwig von Bertalanffy suggested
that couples might have a homeostatic emotional set point, just as
our metabolism regulates our weight around a particular set point.
That metaphor has since been taken up by many family theorists.
Mr. Gottman and his colleagues believe, however, that the
metaphor of homeostasis is misleading, because couples tend to have
multiple set points, not just one. "Volatile" couples, for example,
tend to shift between highly positive and highly negative exchanges,
without stopping in between on some neutral ground. "The concept of
homeostasis doesn't easily describe the potential existence of
sudden, catastrophic change," the authors write, "or any other
'bifurcations' in which the system has two possible paths it can
take, depending on the critical threshold values of the parameters.
So again we need mathematics."
Moreover, says Mr. Gottman,
"The mathematical model draws you to doing certain types of
experiments that you wouldn't have thought of doing without the
model." The nonlinear equations suggest particular ways for
therapists to teach distressed couples to snap out of it when they
find themselves in a negative steady state. Mr. Gottman and his
colleagues have recently tried several such techniques. In one of
these experiments, the scholars trained people to short-circuit
their feelings of contempt by thinking and talking about their
partners' admirable qualities before beginning a difficult
conversation.
With Angie and Dave, Mr. Gottman and his
colleagues used a method they term "dreams within conflict." In this
technique, the therapist encourages the couple to discuss the
"hidden life dreams" that often lie behind conflicts over prosaic
subjects such as money. Rather than encouraging conflict resolution
or "active listening," which more orthodox schools of marital
therapy emphasize, Mr. Gottman wants couples to find ways of
validating each other's deepest interests and anxieties. Angie, for
example, worries that marriage is eroding her earlier sense of
herself as a "free spirit," while Dave maintains a self-image as a
level-headed family provider. Teaching couples to talk to each other
about such anxieties, the authors write, can help "change the
direction of [their] influence functions," by breaking the downward
cycle of difficult conversations: contempt followed by defensiveness
followed by withdrawal.
The goal of these therapeutic
interventions is quite limited, according to Mr. Gottman. "We want
to see if we can make their next conversation on the same topic
better than their last one." He hopes to learn how to quickly give
couples cognitive and behavior skills that will prevent their
conversations from cascading into negativity.
Whether any of
this leads to lasting innovations in real-world marriage therapy
remains to be seen. Robert L. Weiss, a professor emeritus of
psychology at the University of Oregon and a pioneer of
observational studies of marriage, says that Mr. Gottman has a
reputation for moving quickly from one enthusiasm to another. "He
finds gems, but then sometimes builds articles around a small amount
of initial evidence." Mr. Weiss believes, however, that the new book
will be an enduring contribution. "My gut feeling is that John will
turn out to be more right than wrong ... he's got some very
interesting constructs here."
Psychological applications of
dynamic nonlinear models are quite rare, according to Steven H.
Strogatz, a professor of theoretical and applied mechanics in the
College of Engineering at Cornell University and the author of the
new book Sync: The Emerging Science of Spontaneous Order
(Theia).
"There's definitely a community of people out there
who are interested in the interaction between two people as a
dynamical system," he says, citing a recent mathematical study of
psychotherapists and their patients. But on the whole, he continues,
such studies still face skepticism. "Biologists, who by definition
don't like math -- they're people who like science but didn't
want to study math, right? -- have been resistant to
mathematical biology, and often wonder whether the equations give
you anything more than a redescription of what you already knew from
common sense. And so far, there are really only perhaps 10 clear-cut
cases where the mathematical modeling has provided fundamentally new
insights."
But even if Mr. Gottman's nonlinear models never
transform marital therapy, Mr. Strogatz says, they are likely to
prove a useful exercise. "The mere fact of writing equations forces
you to be clear about your hypotheses. With nonlinear modeling
there's nothing hidden."
Mr. Gottman retired from the
university last fall and has established an independent laboratory
in Seattle called the Relationship Research Institute. For his next
project, which he calls "bringing baby home," Mr. Gottman is
studying the effects of newborns on married couples' emotional
equilibriums. Because these models will involve three actors rather
than two, he will be able to experiment with new mathematical tools.
In a declaration that will probably come as no surprise to the
families in his study, he says, "Adding the baby allows you to get
into chaos theory."
QUANTIFYING
CONVERSATIONS
John M. Gottman and his
colleagues have developed "influence functions" to illustrate how
the partners in various types of couples influence each other's
moods over the course of a difficult conversation. The horizontal
axis represents a range of verbal and facial expressions -- from
highly critical and contemptuous (on the left) to highly supportive
and affectionate (on the right).

The vertical axis represents the degree to which a
person is influenced positively (up) or negatively (down) by his or
her spouse's behavior. For example, the husband in a "validating"
couple tends to be strongly influenced (in a negative direction) by
his wife's negative behavior: That line has a slope of 0.31. The
husband in a "conflict avoiding" couple, on the other hand, actually
tends to behave slightly more positively when his wife is negative:
That line has a slope of 0.05.

Both "validating" couples and "conflict avoiding"
couples tend to have marriages that are stable and long lasting.
Couples are in trouble, though, when they have mismatched influence
functions. Consider the "hostile detached" couple, where the slopes
for the husband's and the wife's influences look very different.
When the husband behaves positively, he has almost no impact (slope
of 0.02) on his detached wife.

SOURCE: The Mathematics of
Marriage: Dynamic Linear Models (MIT Press); graphic adapted By
Dave Allen
http://chronicle.com Section: Research & Publishing Volume
49, Issue 33, Page A14
|
Copyright © 2003 by The
Chronicle of Higher Education
|